The
effects of brain injury
The effects of brain injury are by and large not understood by
the world. The person who manifests symptoms of lack of motivation
is more than likely going to be accused of laziness. In a world
which is a cultural construction, the person with cognitive deficits
is more likely to have their behaviour interpreted in cultural
terms. The fact that so many people with brain injury end up in
prison is one of the most obvious pieces of evidence of this fact.
A good neuropsychology assessment is critical in identifying the
origin and extent of cognitive changes for the person with brain
injury, but it is not possible to force the world to read these
reports and have any kind of understanding of their implications.
It is worth drawing attention to the similarity of the words used
to describe the deficits which are of biological origin and those
which have a cultural basis to appreciate why the work of the
neuropsychologist is so important for promoting a real understanding
of what the person with brain injury has to live with.
There are…… signs of a defective capacity for
self-control or self-direction such as emotional lability or
flattening, a heightened tendency to irritability and excitability,
impulsivity, erratic carelessness, rigidity and difficulty in
making shifts in attention and in ongoing behaviour. Deterioration
in personal grooming and cleanliness may also distinguish these
patients…..there is impaired capacity to initiate activity,
decreased or absent motivation (anergia), and defects in planning
and carrying out the activity sequences that make up goal directed
behaviours. Patients without significant impairment of receptive
or expressive functions who suffer primarily from these kinds
of control defects are often mistakenly judged to be malingering,
lazy or spoiled, psychiatrically disturbed, or - if this kind
of defect appears following a legally compensable brain injury
- exhibiting a 'compensation neurosis' that some interested
persons may believe will disappear when the patient's legal
claim has been settled. (Lezak 1995).
The neuropsychologist is the person best placed to give an accurate
diagnosis and possible prognosis for the brain disorder. The prognosis
should include some idea of the time scale over which neurologic
recovery can be expected, together with an eventual outcome, where
the level of recovery is likely to reach a plateau (Mills, Cassidy,
Katz 1997). A diagnosis of traumatic head injury is not enough
to give this kind of information, and it needs further elaboration
of the kind provided in the report quoted above in the Introduction
to this thesis:
Mr Williams was admitted to Rathnew Hospital in a deeply
unconscious state after a motorcycle accident in 27 December
1991. At admission his Glasgow Coma score was 5/15 and a CT
head scan showed brain swelling and diffuse brain injury with
numerous small haemorrhages in the right hemisphere and in the
left occipital lobe. He remained unconscious until about the
20th January. …On 12th February 1992 he uttered his first
words. A CT scan in February 1992 showed mild cortical atrophy
and this picture has been present in subsequent scans. He received
a very severe closed head injury as indicated by the duration
of coma and his period of post-traumatic amnesia which was estimated
by clinical staff as up to 3 months.
This level of diagnosis allows one to predict the kinds of outcomes
that will ensue and these can be summarised (Mills, Cassidy, Katz
1997):
Cognitive problems involve deficits in attention, memory,
and higher-level regulatory or executive processes. Even after
the basic elements of attention resolve with the passage of
the confusional state, more complex attentional problems remain.
Patients usually have a slowed rate of mental processing, increased
susceptibility to distracting interference, and difficulty dividing
attention among tow or more tasks. The functional implications
of these problems include slowed completion of more complex
everyday activities (eg meal preparation) and difficulty concentrating
on more than one thing at a time (eg talking and driving).
Although dense amnesia is not usually seen in uncomplicated
injuries, memory problems remain. Patients are often forgetful,
have trouble with ‘automatic,’ passive memory and
have less effective learning strategies. Even when tests of
general intellectual function have normalized, higher cognitive
processes such as those associated with the frontal systems
remain dysfunctional. These so-called executive problems present
during tasks that require flexible thinking, deviation from
routine, and self-reflection. Due to such problems, planning
an alternate route when encountering heavy traffic or efficiently
organizing a number of errands may become overwhelming. Frontal
system dysfunction also leads to the emotional and behavioral
problems that are so common following brain injury. With the
loss of orbital frontal supervisory control over emotions and
appetitive drive states, impulsivity, disinhibition, and personality
changes occur. However, when lateral frontal dysfunction predominates,
patients are apathetic, unconcerned (abulic), and unmotivated.
Limbic disconnection syndromes produce mood disorders,
irritability, and on occasion, outright aggression.
This description gives many of the expected outcomes for someone
with a severe diffuse traumatic brain injury at the end of the
recovery stage. The timeline for recovery for patients with severe
diffuse injury extends over years, however there comes a point
where they are ready for community re-entry. The most well known
description of the epochs of recovery for this type of injury
is the Los Ranchos scale (Hagen et al 1972). At this point, which
equates to levels VII and VIII on the Los Ranchos scale, there
are other factors which become more important and neurology simply
becomes one factor among many which needs to be considered -
This final stage of recovery is better delineated by psychosocial
criteria than by specific neurologic milestones. As such, the
impact of the underlying neurology on recovery lessens and an
individual’s premorbid personality and social resources
become increasingly important in predicting outcome. Premorbid
developmental, medical or psychiatric disorders and the support
of the person’s social network all play increasingly important
roles in determining how fast and how far he or she will progress
(Mills, Cassidy, Katz 1997).
These authors then proceed to say that this progression will
lead to further cognitive improvement, which will in turn lead
to community integration and an new self awareness. They indicate
a complex inter-relationship between cognitive functions and what
the person goes on to do, without elucidating the details. I suspect
that work could be done on this relationship between the brain
and normal behaviour, but it would not be useful. We already have
ways of describing degrees of competence in normal behaviour and
we no longer need the neuropsychologist at this point. I return
to Ryle (1949) for corroboration.
The classification and diagnosis of exhibitions of our
mental impotences require specialized research methods. The
explanation of the exhibitions of our mental competences often
requires nothing but ordinary good sense, or it may require
the specialized methods of economists, scholars, strategists,
and examiners (p. 398)
The kind of knowledge that I need in order to ‘know how
to go on’ does not require a degree in psychology. I do
not need to know psychological theory and have an ability to apply
it in order to understand whether a cupboard door has been hung
competently, or a table made correctly. However, some rudimentary
idea of carpentry might be useful if I am to judge his performance
as successful or otherwise.
Understanding is a part of knowing how. The knowledge that
is required for understanding intelligent performances of a
specific kind is some degree of competence in performances of
that kind. The competent critic of prose-style, experimental
technique, or embroidery, must at least know how to write, experiment
or sew. Whether or not he has also learned some psychology matters
about as much as whether he has learned any chemistry, neurology
or economics. These studies may in certain circumstances assist
his appreciation of what he is criticizing; but the one necessary
condition is that he has some mastery of the art or procedure,
examples of which he is to appraise. For one person to see the
jokes that another makes, the one thing he must have is a sense
of humour and even that special brand of sense of humour of
which those jokes are exercises. (Ryle 1949 p. 53)
The occupational therapist takes the effects of brain injury
into account
Neurology is therefore extremely important in providing an understanding
of the underlying deficits which the person with brain injury
has to live with. It is particularly important that there should
be an understanding of these deficits because they have so many
cultural manifestations which would cause the person with brain
injury to be condemned if they are not understood. Improvement
in the underlying cognitive deficit can continue for years, especially
after a severe diffuse injury. However there comes a time when
this improvement is irrelevant in relation to all the other factors
which are impacting on him. At this point, while the underlying
deficit continue to be held in mind, the progression that he makes
cannot be considered in neurological terms. His performance at
this point needs to be described and judged in terms of competencies
rather than in terms of deficits.
At this point one begins to reach for a concept of the mind which
might accommodate this particular kind of knowing. Ryle provides
this in one of his more controversial theories, which yet seems
to exactly describe what I am trying to say.
The statement ‘the mind is its own place’,
as theorists might construe it, is not true, for the mind is
not even a metaphorical ‘place’. On the contrary,
the chessboard, the platform, the scholar’s desk, the
judge’s bench, the lorry-driver’s seat, the studio
and the football field are among its places. These are where
people work and play stupidly or intelligently. ‘Mind’
is not the name of another person, working or frolicking behind
an impenetrable screen; it is not the name of another place
where work is done or games are played; and it is not the name
of another tool with which work is done, or another appliance
with which games are played.
I find myself agreeing with Ryle’s notion that our mind
exists in the places where it is engaged. It seems to echo Marx’s
idea that of work that “the blacksmith forges and the product
is a forging”. Neurology provides a way of looking at the
mind/brain, but I find that it is not the way that I look at the
mind of someone who is grasping at achieving the fullest expression
of his humanity possible.
I start with a definition of occupation, and then look at literature
which indicates that by acting on the external world man changes
his nature. This world is something which we hold in common and
I emphasise this commonality throughout. I then talk about alienation
and idleness as two particular manifestations of the need for
occupation.
The notion of being well occupied can best be described by describing
actual engagement in particular activities. However, it is possible
to outline some things which are common to most manifestations
of being well occupied. It is a fact that we are well able and
used to judging what people do already, we understand behaviour
without needing any special qualification beyond belonging to
a common culture. The predicament of needing occupation could
be described as not ‘knowing how to go on’ and in
this section I describe what form this ‘knowing might take.
I say that it will take the form of the activity which is being
done, whether that be playing a game of chess, or making a table.
Some examples are given of how an activity might be analysed when
using this framework. A critical part of any activity is the finishing
of it and there are many things associated with a thing that is
finished, compared to things which are not finished. I try to
describe some of these differences and the associations that come
from the satisfactory completion of some types of occupation.
I move then to a theory which says that our task is to find our
‘real work’ and that we create a world of hope and
a space for action. Within this space we can reflect back the
meaning of what we have done, and our real individuality begins
to take form. I give some small vignettes, which illustrate some
of the ways that this framework has been described in the literature.
Then I explain they reasons why I have not focussed on the very
extensive literature on neurology to answer my research questions.
This point is touched on when I define what I mean by ‘common
sense’ and how an exclusive focus on deficits can isolate
the person with brain injury. I strongly believe that good diagnosis
and prognosis is essential, but even in the neuropsychology literature
it is said that psychosocial criteria are more important than
specific neurologic milestones, in the longterm. I therefore conclude
with a return to the idea that the kind of understanding that
is needed here is an understanding of occupation. By keeping the
focus on occupation it is possible to arrive at a theory of the
mind which is congruent with it.
Next page: The need for occupation
|
Site
Links
Home
page
Abstract
Introduction
Methods and Ethics
Guestbook (to be enabled soon)
Brain damage stories-
Stories intro
Story 1 - The accident
Story 2 - The OT arrives
Story 3 - The CD rack
Story 4 - The troll
Story 5 - The door
Story 6 - At work
Story 7 - The letterbox
Story 8 - Employment
Occupation in Literature -
Literature intro
Occupation
Alienation
Being "well occupied"
The practitioner / OT
The person with brain injury
Discussion -
The need for occupation
Becoming well occupied
Facilitation
Ethical concerns
Occupation and neurology
Future research
Conclusion
Works cited
Bibliography
Brain
injury and head injury resources
Occupational
Therapy and carer resources
OT
jobs
Rehab equipment
Physical rehab
Brain
injury web sites
General
brain injury resources
Organizations and programs
USA Brain injury association chapters
Headway branches
Brain injury Research
Brain injury support and chat
Brain injury mailing lists
Personal stories
Residential programs and similar
services
|